Preferences for honesty can support cooperation

dc.contributor.authorSzekely, Aron
dc.contributor.authorBruner, David
dc.contributor.authorSteinmo, Sven
dc.contributor.authorVolintiru, Clara
dc.contributor.authorAndrighetto, Giulia
dc.contributor.authorTodor, Arpad
dc.date.accessioned2025-09-10T07:28:09Z
dc.date.available2025-09-10T07:28:09Z
dc.date.issued2023-04-16
dc.descriptionThis is an Open Access article under the CC-By 4.0 license available at: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/bdm.2328 This article is free available on Wiley Online Library platform at: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/bdm.2328 The author Arpad Todor is affiliated to National University of Political Studies and Public Administration Bucharest, Faculty of Political Science.
dc.description.abstractMany collective action problems are inherently linked to honesty. By deciding to behave honestly, people contribute to solving the collective action problem. We use a laboratory experiment from two sites (n = 331 and n = 319) to test whether honest preferences can drive cooperation and whether these preferences can be differentially activated by framing. Subjects participate in an asymmetric information variant of the public goods game in one of two treatments that vary only in their wording: The Contribution Frame uses a standard public good game framing, while in the Honesty Frame, words aimed to trigger honesty are used. We measure subjects' honesty in three ways using the (i) sender–receiver task, (ii) the die-roll task, and (iii) self-reported honesty levels and account for other-regarding preferences and social norms to disentangle key alternative motives. We find that all three measures of honesty preferences robustly predict contributions, as do other-regarding preferences and empirical expectations but not normative expectations. Additionally, honesty preferences predict contributions in the Honesty Frame but not in the Contribution Frame, although the difference between these is not consistently significant. Finally, we find no differences in average cooperation across the treatments.
dc.identifier.citationSzekely, A., Bruner, D., Steinmo, S., Todor, A., Volintiru, C., & Andrighetto, G. (2023). Preferences for honesty can support cooperation. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 36(4), e2328. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.2328
dc.identifier.issn0894-3257
dc.identifier.issn1099-0771
dc.identifier.otherhttps://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.2328
dc.identifier.urihttps://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/bdm.2328
dc.identifier.urihttps://debdfdsi.snspa.ro/handle/123456789/1139
dc.language.isoen
dc.publisherJohn Wiley & Sons
dc.subjectSocial norms
dc.subjectSocial preferences
dc.subjectLaboratory experiment
dc.subjectCooperation
dc.titlePreferences for honesty can support cooperation
dc.typeArticle

Files

Original bundle

Now showing 1 - 1 of 1
No Thumbnail Available
Name:
Contents_Preferences.pdf
Size:
1.7 MB
Format:
Adobe Portable Document Format

License bundle

Now showing 1 - 1 of 1
No Thumbnail Available
Name:
license.txt
Size:
1.71 KB
Format:
Item-specific license agreed to upon submission
Description: